Adaptive Resonance Theory


Advertisements


Introduction

One of the nice features of human memory is its ability to learn many new
things without necessarily forgetting things learned in the past. A frequently
cited example is the ability to recognize your parents even if you have not seen
them for some time and have learned many new faces in the interim. It would
be highly desirable if we could impart this same capability to an Artificial Neural Networks. Most
neural networks will tend to forget old
information if we attempt to add new information incrementally.
When developing an artificial neural network to perform a particular pattern-classification operation,
we typically proceed by gathering a set of exemplars, or training patterns,
then using these exemplars to train the system.

During the training, information
is encoded in the system by the adjustment of weight values. Once the training
is deemed to be adequate, the system is ready to be put into production, and no
additional weight modification is permitted.
This operational scenario is acceptable provided the problem domain has
well-defined boundaries and is stable. Under such conditions, it is usually
possible to define an adequate set of training inputs for whatever problem is
being solved. Unfortunately, in many realistic situations, the environment is
neither bounded nor stable.
Consider a simple example. Suppose you intend to train a backpropagation to recognize
the silhouettes of a certain class of aircraft. The appropriate images can be
collected and used to train the network, which is potentially a time-consuming
task depending on the size of the network required. After the network has
learned successfully to recognize all of the aircraft, the training period is ended
and no further modification of the weights is allowed.
If, at some future time, another aircraft in the same class becomes operational,
you may wish to add its silhouette to the store of knowledge in your neural network. To do this, you would have to retrain the network with the new pattern
plus all of the previous patterns. Training on only the new silhouette could result
in the network learning that pattern quite well, but forgetting previously learned
patterns. Although retraining may not take as long as the initial training, it still
could require a significant investment.

The Adaptative Resonance Theory: ART

In 1976, Grossberg (Grossberg, 1976) introduced a model for explaining biological phenomena.
The model has three crucial properties:

  1. a normalisation of the total network activity. Biological systems are usually very adaptive
    to large changes in their environment. For example, the human eye can adapt itself to
    large variations in light intensities;
  2. contrast enhancement of input patterns. The awareness of subtle di erences in input
    patterns can mean a lot in terms of survival. Distinguishing a hiding panther from a
    resting one makes all the diference in the world. The mechanism used here is contrast
    enhancement;
  3. short-term memory (STM) storage of the contrast-enhanced pattern. Before the input
    pattern can be decoded, it must be stored in the short-term memory. The long-term
    memory (LTM) implements an arousal mechanism (i.e., the classi cation), whereas the
    STM is used to cause gradual changes in the LTM.

The system consists of two layers, F1 and F2, which are connected to each other via the
LTM

The input pattern is received at F1, whereas classi cation takes place in
F2. As mentioned before, the input is not directly classified. First a characterisation takes place by means of extracting features, giving rise to activation in the feature representation field. The
expectations, residing in the LTM connections, translate the input pattern to a categorisation
in the category representation field. The classi cation is compared to the expectation of the
network, which resides in the LTM weights from F2 to F1. If there is a match, the expectations
are strengthened, otherwise the classification is rejected.

ART1: The simplified neural network model

The ART1 simplified model consists of two layers of binary neurons (with values 1 and 0), called
F1 (the comparison layer) and F2 (the recognition layer)

Each neuron in F1
is connected to all neurons in F2 via the continuous-valued forward long term memory (LTM)
Wf , and vice versa via the binary-valued backward LTM Wb. The other modules are gain 1
and 2 (G1 and G2), and a reset module.
Each neuron in the comparison layer receives three inputs: a component of the input pattern,
a component of the feedback pattern, and a gain G1. A neuron outputs a 1 if and only if at
least three of these inputs are high: the ‘two-thirds rule.’
The neurons in the recognition layer each compute the inner product of their incoming
(continuous-valued) weights and the pattern sent over these connections. The winning neuron
then inhibits all the other neurons via lateral inhibition.
Gain 2 is the logical ‘or’ of all the elements in the input pattern x.
Gain 1 equals gain 2, except when the feedback pattern from F2 contains any 1; then it is
forced to zero.
Finally, the reset signal is sent to the active neuron in F2 if the input vector x and the
output of F1 di er by more than some vigilance level.

Operation

The network starts by clamping the input at F1. Because the output of F2 is zero, G1 and G2
are both on and the output of F1 matches its input.The pattern is sent to F2, and in F2 one neuron becomes active. This signal is then sent
back over the backward LTM, which reproduces a binary pattern at F1. Gain 1 is inhibited,
and only the neurons in F1 which receive a ‘one’ from both x and F2 remain active.
If there is a substantial mismatch between the two patterns, the reset signal will inhibit the
neuron in F2 and the process is repeated.

  1. Initialisation:

    where N is the number of neurons in F1, M the number of neurons in F2, 0  i < N,
    and 0 ≤ j

  2. Apply the new input pattern x:
  3. compute the activation values y0 of the neurons in F2:
  4. select the winning neuron k (0 ≤ k
  5. vigilance test: if

    where . denotes inner product, go to step 7, else go to step 6. Note that essentially is the inner product,which will be large if and near to each other;

  6. neuron k is disabled from further activity. Go to step 3;
  7. Set for all l, 0 ≤ l < N:

  8. re-enable all neurons in F2 and go to step 2.



An example of the behaviour of the Carpenter Grossberg network for letter patterns.
The binary input patterns on the left were applied sequentially. On the right the stored patterns (i.e.,
the weights of Wb for the first four output units) are shown.

ART1: The original model

In later work, Carpenter and Grossberg (Carpenter & Grossberg, 1987a, 1987b) present several
neural network models to incorporate parts of the complete theory. We will only discuss the
first model, ART1.
The network incorporates a follow-the-leader clustering algorithm (Hartigan, 1975). This
algorithm tries to fit each new input pattern in an existing class. If no matching class can be
found, i.e., the distance between the new pattern and all existing classes exceeds some threshold,
a new class is created containing the new pattern.
The novelty in this approach is that the network is able to adapt to new incoming patterns,
while the previous memory is not corrupted. In most neural networks, such as the backpropagation
network, all patterns must be taught sequentially; the teaching of a new pattern
might corrupt the weights for all previously learned patterns. By changing the structure of the
network rather than the weights, ART1 overcomes this problem.